dc.contributor.author | Lundquist, Bjørn | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2022-05-25T12:43:58Z | |
dc.date.available | 2022-05-25T12:43:58Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2014-08-22 | |
dc.description.abstract | As was discussed in the section on verb particles in active clauses, the verb particle always
precedes the direct objects in Swedish, while it often follows the direct object in the other
Nordic varieties, especially when the direct object is a nonstressed pronoun. When it comes
to simple reflexive direct objects (sig)
[1]
, the pattern in Swedish is more complex. Depending
on verb class, the particle either precedes (1a) or follows (1b) sig. Some particle verbs allow
both options, often with slightly different interpretations: when the particle follows the
reflexive, the particle seems to modify the event rather than the following state (1c). This is
exemplified in (1) below (curly brackets indicate possible positions for the particle):
[2] | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Lundquist B. Verb-particles: placement with respect to reflexive pronouns. Nordic Atlas of Language Structures (NALS) Journal. 2014;1:127-135 | en_US |
dc.identifier.cristinID | FRIDAID 1206838 | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.5617/nals.5374 | |
dc.identifier.issn | 2387-2667 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/10037/25289 | |
dc.language.iso | eng | en_US |
dc.publisher | University of Oslo | en_US |
dc.relation.journal | Nordic Atlas of Language Structures (NALS) Journal | |
dc.relation.uri | http://www.tekstlab.uio.no/nals/chapter_text/6/particlerefl.pdf | |
dc.rights.accessRights | openAccess | en_US |
dc.rights.holder | Copyright 2014 The Author(s) | en_US |
dc.title | Verb-particles: placement with respect to reflexive pronouns | en_US |
dc.type.version | publishedVersion | en_US |
dc.type | Journal article | en_US |
dc.type | Tidsskriftartikkel | en_US |
dc.type | Peer reviewed | en_US |