Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorKolb, Nadine
dc.contributor.authorMitrofanova, Natalia
dc.contributor.authorWestergaard, Marit Kristine Richardsen
dc.date.accessioned2023-10-20T11:22:10Z
dc.date.available2023-10-20T11:22:10Z
dc.date.issued2023-09-22
dc.description.abstractLanguage learners typically experience cross-linguistic influence (CLI) from one or several previously acquired languages when acquiring an additional language. Learners of a second language (L2) are influenced by their native languages in all language domains ranging from phonology, lexicon, and morphosyntax, to semantics, discourse, and pragmatics. In third (and subsequent) language (L3/ Ln) acquisition, however, there is more than one potential source of influence, as the learner already knows at least two languages, which both have the potential to influence subsequent language acquisition. This is the reason why existing models of L3/Ln language acquisition have mainly focused on issues related to CLI: (i) the source of CLI (L1, L2, both languages, typologically closest language, language of communication), (ii) the type of CLI (only facilitative or both facilitative and non-facilitative), and (iii) the extent of CLI (wholesale or property-by-property or hybrid). Different models attribute CLI to different factors, and there is still no consensus in multilingual acquisition research. According to existing models of L3 acquisition, the following factors may be the source of CLI: Order and/or manner of acquisition (The L2 Status Factor, e.g., Bardel & Falk 2007; L1 Transfer, e.g., Jin 2009), language use (The Language of Communication Model, e.g., Fallah et al., 2016; Fallah & Jabbari 2018) or some kind of structural similarity (The Typological Primacy Model, Rothman, 2015; The Linguistic Proximity Model, Westergaard et al., 2017; Westergaard 2021a, b; The Scalpel Model, Slabakova, 2017). The five articles in this issue zoom in on this last factor, structural similarity, specifically in phonology and morphosyntax, which are examined at early stages of L3 acquisition as well as in L3 development.en_US
dc.identifier.citationKolb, Mitrofanova, Westergaard. Structural similarity in third language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. 2023en_US
dc.identifier.cristinIDFRIDAID 2178445
dc.identifier.doi10.1075/lab.23037.kol
dc.identifier.issn1879-9264
dc.identifier.issn1879-9272
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10037/31592
dc.language.isoengen_US
dc.publisherJohn Benjamins Publishing Companyen_US
dc.relation.journalLinguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
dc.relation.projectIDEC/H2020: 882311en_US
dc.relation.projectIDinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/EC/H2020/882311/EU/Investigating Proxies for Understanding Trajectories: Heritage Language Maintenance and Child Second Language Acquisition in Refugee Contexts/INPUT/en_US
dc.rights.accessRightsopenAccessen_US
dc.rights.holderCopyright 2023 The Author(s)en_US
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0en_US
dc.rightsAttribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0)en_US
dc.titleStructural similarity in third language acquisitionen_US
dc.type.versionpublishedVersionen_US
dc.typeJournal articleen_US
dc.typeTidsskriftartikkelen_US
dc.typePeer revieweden_US


File(s) in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following collection(s)

Show simple item record

Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0)
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0)